Rules of thumb can lead to politicians misjudging voters' reactions

Decision-makers need to be able to judge how voters will react to a policy. It is important for their continued electoral success, and for the success of the policy. Yet, write Chris Butler and Barbara Vis, systematic biases often lead policymakers to misjudge voters’ reactions to their policy decisions – with damaging consequences

How do politicians and their advisers form judgments about voters’ opinions? Take, for example, Keir Starmer’s recent reluctance to position the Labour Party on the side of striking workers, or Conservative politicians’ propensity to talk about trans issues. These examples illustrate how policymakers may not always pay enough attention to what voters really want. If politicians fail to respond to the opinions of the electorate, voters may lose trust in democratic institutions.

Making accurate judgments is hard

Policy responsiveness theories generally assume that policymakers can make such accurate judgments. In practice, however, this is extremely difficult. Opposition framing of an issue or the unforeseen outcomes from a policy can affect voters’ response. Moreover, judgments are usually made under severe time pressure, with decision-makers facing an overload of information.

Existing research shows that in such circumstances, decision-makers are likely to use rules of thumb, also known as heuristics. Such simplified decision rules can be useful for making assessments. However, they also occasionally lead to biases – systematic deviations from comprehensive rationality – and thus to misjudgments.

Under severe time pressure, decision-makers often resort to rules of thumb – heuristics – to make assessments

It is plausible that such biases are present in judgments of voters’ reactions to policy. Previous research shows that policymakers often misjudge voters’ policy preferences. However, it has not yet been investigated whether biases play a role here.

Important decisions with negative electoral consequences

In a recent European Political Science article, we use two major political decisions from the United Kingdom to show how biases can indeed contribute to misjudgments of voters’ reactions to policy decisions.

First, we looked at the New Labour government's decision to abstain from temporary controls on migration from countries that joined the European Union in 2004. This decision caused a continuous increase in immigration to the UK during New Labour's time in office (until 2010).

We also examined the Liberal Democrats' decision to accept a tripling of tuition fees as part of its 2010 coalition agreement with the Conservatives. This U-turn came about after the party had previously advocated a complete abolition of such fees.

In both high-profile cases we examined, political parties suffered reputationally because they misjudged how voters would react

Making accurate judgments was particularly important because the high profile and salience of both issues made it highly likely that voters would update their judgments of the parties on the basis of the parties' decisions. Yet in both cases, the parties suffered reputationally because they misjudged how voters would react.

Examples of biases: availability and representativeness

In the two cases, we identified 12 misjudgments about electoral responses. We can attribute these misjudgments to a bias related to one of two general-purpose heuristics identified in Tversky and Kahneman’s work on heuristics and biases. People use the availability heuristic when assessing the likelihood of an occurrence by focusing on the ease with which people can recall instances of it. In the immigration case, for example, policymakers paid more attention to asylum seekers than to economic immigration. Since there was considerable media focus on asylum seekers, we identified this as a more salient concern to voters than economic immigration. However, voters reacted negatively to the rise in legal migration – and this contributed to Labour losing the 2010 election.

We also recognised an example of a bias related to the representativeness heuristic in the tuition fee case. This happens when we calculate the likelihood of an event occurring by comparing how much the possible event resembles an existing event. In 2003, New Labour had made a similar U-turn, yet still managed to win the 2005 election. This misled some Liberal Democrats into thinking they might get away with their own tuition-fee U-turn.

Both cases involved an about-face over tuition fees. But, crucially, there were significant differences in terms of how much the parties relied on the student vote, and for some voters, the Liberal Democrats, having formed a coalition with the Conservative Party, already appeared to be lacking in principles. All this meant the Liberal Democrats suffered far greater electoral damage over tuition fees than had New Labour a few years earlier.

After forming a coalition government with the UK Conservative Party led by David Cameron (R), Liberal Democrat leader Nick Clegg (L) was forced into a humiliating U-turn on his party's manifesto pledge to abolish student tuition fees

Misjudging voters

Now, of course, misjudgments about voter reactions to policy are not solely attributable to biases about availability or representativeness. However, our findings show that these biases do play a role – one that has been overlooked in existing political science research.

Our findings show that biases about availability or representativeness play a role that is overlooked in existing research

We should pay close attention to such biases in future research on decision-makers’ misjudgments of voters. We could, for example, analyse whether availability-biases occur more or less frequently in political decisions that receive less media attention.

Our research also shows that decision-makers typically expect voters to respond rationally to policy outcomes. Yet a glance at discussions in the public arena shows this isn't necessarily the case. This, too, deserves further research.

This article presents the views of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the ECPR or the Editors of The Loop.

Contributing Authors

photograph of Chris Butler Chris Butler Lecturer in Politics, University of Manchester More by this author
photograph of Barbara Vis Barbara Vis Professor of Politics & Governance, Utrecht University School of Governance More by this author

Share Article

Republish Article

We believe in the free flow of information Republish our articles for free, online or in print, under a Creative Commons license.

Creative Commons License

Comments

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

The Loop

Cutting-edge analysis showcasing the work of the political science discipline at its best.
Read more
THE EUROPEAN CONSORTIUM FOR POLITICAL RESEARCH
Advancing Political Science
© 2024 European Consortium for Political Research. The ECPR is a charitable incorporated organisation (CIO) number 1167403 ECPR, Harbour House, 6-8 Hythe Quay, Colchester, CO2 8JF, United Kingdom.
linkedin facebook pinterest youtube rss twitter instagram facebook-blank rss-blank linkedin-blank pinterest youtube twitter instagram